Tuesday, July 14, 2020

 Language is a telling clue to 
unacknowledged racial attitudes

 Overt racism is declining, but studies show 
that unconscious bias remains widespread



 “When the looting starts, the shooting starts,” tweeted 
Donald Trump (invoking a slogan from the 1960s), when unrest 
broke out after the killing of George Floyd. “Thugs”, the 
president wrote, were disrespecting Mr Floyd’s memory. He 
almost always use ”Thugs” when some matter of race is at 
issue, either the treatment of African-Americans or in 
reference to “illegals” and “gang members”, implicitly Latinos.

 Are “looting” and “thugs” a kind of racist code? Many people 
detect a “dog-whistle” which, without saying anything 
explicitly racist, the president intends listeners to hear. 
Mr Trump forcefully disagrees. In 2015 he tweeted: “They now 
say using the word ‘thug’ is, like so many other words, not 
politically correct (even though Obama uses it). It is racist - 
bull!” Plenty of people approve of calling thugs thugs, and 
object to being labelled racists for doing so.

 The intentional dog-whistle is not unknown. But people can 
allude to race without realising it, too. Explicit racism is 
declining in America, as a pile of evidence shows. For 
instance, there is a widespread reluctance to admit racist 
attitudes that many Americans were once perfectly comfortable 
with. On the other hand, unconscious and semi-conscious 
prejudice is alive and well. When people use racially linked 
language, without overt slurs or other racist tropes, 
linguists call it “racialisation”.

 Some ingenious research has teased out the links between 
prejudice and language. A classic method for fingering 
implicit bias asks subjects to take a quick-fire, button-pushing
test that associates positive or negative words with white and 
black faces. In one such study (at implicit.harvard.edu), 58% 
of online test-takers are slower to associate positive 
language with black faces than with white ones. Just 14% 
display the opposite tendency.

 Or consider more deliberate forms of language, formulated by 
people who are generally unlikely to consider themselves 
racist, journalists. A study by Dana Mastro of the University 
of California at Santa Barbara looked at hundreds of news 
reports in three big American papers (the New York Times, the 
Los Angeles Times and USA Today) about professional athletes 
accused of crimes. Such articles were more accusatory of the 
alleged perpetrators when they were black. They were more 
likely to provide context and humanising detail when the 
accused were white.

 Praise can be racialised, too. Crystal Dunn Soubrier, a 
black player for America’s women’s football team, wrote 
recently that commentators attribute her ability to play in 
several positions to athleticism, never to her reading or 
study of the game. This seems a persistent pattern in 
sportswriting. Kelly Wright, a doctoral student of linguistics 
at the University of Michigan, used machine-learning to 
predict an athlete’s race based on words that appear in an 
article containing their name. Purely relying on the language 
of news reports, the model was able to predict that Ronda 
Rousey, a wrestler, was white with a 96% certainty, whereas 
there was only a 3% probability that Eric Berry, a National 
Football League player, was white. So it went with other 
athletes; the algorithm’s guesses were almost always loaded 
starkly towards black or white.

 When racialisation happens, words do not change their 
dictionary definition; rather, they take on associations 
with other words. Brains are statistical machines, in which 
learning involves a gradual strengthening or weakening of 
different synapses. Introduce one concept and related concepts 
are “primed”, or made more quickly available to the 
consciousness. Just because these associations are hard to 
prove conclusively does not make them less real.

 The difference between “racist” and “racialised” helps explain 
why Americans often talk fruitlessly past each other when 
discussing words such as “thug”. With overt racism waning, it 
has become painfully obvious to some people—though not all—that 
the submerged part of the iceberg, implicit racial beliefs and 
associations, plays a bigger role than was once realised. 
Overcoming those is particularly difficult because of their 
semi-conscious nature.

 A long struggle has made “racist” one of the worst things 
you can be. That counts as a great success, but the corollary 
is that the remaining problems are hard to talk about. Some 
fear being branded racist just for trying. Seeing the bias 
in even innocently intended language is a first step towards 
understanding that there is still work to do.

Also check: 

No comments:

Post a Comment